Definition/Introduction
The Haidinger brush phenomenon, also known as Haidinger brushes, is a distinctive and intriguing visual phenomenon that allows humans to perceive the polarization of light. This entoptic phenomenon is named after Austrian scientist Wilhelm Karl von Haidinger, who first reported the phenomenon in 1844. The Haidinger brush phenomenon is an elliptical, yellowish-blue pattern against a blue or white background when viewing polarized light.[1] The visualization of Haidinger brushes is a way of observing and potentially monitoring the density and condition of xanthophyll pigment in the macula and, in this manner identifying and tracking macular disease.[2]
Issues of Concern
Register For Free And Read The Full Article
- Search engine and full access to all medical articles
- 10 free questions in your specialty
- Free CME/CE Activities
- Free daily question in your email
- Save favorite articles to your dashboard
- Emails offering discounts
Learn more about a Subscription to StatPearls Point-of-Care
Issues of Concern
Haidinger brushes are an entoptic phenomenon that offers an intriguing insight into the intricate workings of the human visual system, particularly regarding its ability to perceive polarized light. This perception largely depends on the interaction between the xanthophyll pigments in the macula and polarized light, creating a phenomenon that individuals can observe directly.[3] The variability in perception, across individual and demographic differences and in conjunction with certain pathophysiological conditions such as age-related macular degeneration, forms an integral part of understanding the nuances of this phenomenon.[4]
The perception of Haidinger brushes is specifically attributed to the dichroism possessed by the xanthophyll pigment in the macula lutea.[2] These pigments can absorb light of specific wavelengths depending on their orientation, creating extrinsic dichroism in the foveal blue cones, which govern the behavior and distribution of oblique rays within the cylindrical geometry of these cones.[5] The high concentration of dichroic pigments uniquely interacts with polarized light. When aligned properly, these pigments can selectively absorb light waves based on their oscillatory direction, thus creating a differential absorption pattern that the brain interprets as Haidinger brushes.[2]
From a research perspective, the phenomenon can serve as a noninvasive probe into the functional integrity of the macular area. Studies investigating the relationship between the perception of Haidinger brushes and conditions like age-related macular degeneration could provide insights into disease detection and progression. Moreover, investigations into the exact mechanisms behind the perception of polarized light by the human visual system help to improve our understanding of retinal physiology and visual processing.
Given the critical role of macular pigment optical density (MPOD) in visual health, it is interesting to note that the ability to perceive Haidinger brushes correlates with the specific MPOD of an individual.[6][7] As such, the phenomenon provides a potential means to gauge the concentration of macular pigment and, consequently, macular health.[8][9]
Research on the role of xanthophyll pigments in the macula has led to their use as a supplement in age-related macular degeneration to increase MPOD. An alteration in the perception of Haidinger brushes could reflect this effect.[6][10] In clinical practice, the phenomenon of Haidinger brushes is not routinely utilized as a diagnostic tool due to its subjective nature and the variability of perception among individuals. However, Haidinger brushes can serve as an interesting clinical sign, demonstrating the dichroism of the xanthophyll pigment found in the macula lutea.[8] As such, any changes in the perception of Haidinger brushes could signal changes in the macular area, enabling it to serve as a self-monitoring tool for some patients.[4]
Haidinger Brushes and Polarized Light
Polarization of light is a property of light waves that describes the orientation of their oscillations. In an unpolarized light beam, the direction of the electric field vector is randomly distributed in all possible directions perpendicular to the direction of propagation. However, the electric field vector oscillates in one specific direction when light is polarized. This can occur through various processes, such as reflection, refraction, or scattering.[11] Polarized light is common in the natural environment and can be observed in various situations; light reflected off a body of water or the sky's light in certain directions relative to the sun. Polarized light is commonly manipulated by sunglasses that aim to reduce glare by blocking horizontally polarized light.[12][13] The human eye typically cannot directly perceive polarization, with the unique phenomenon of Haidinger brushes demonstrating an exception to this rule.
The blue-yellow coloration and characteristic bow-tie shape of Haidinger brushes are the visual representation of polarized light. The ability to perceive Haidinger brushes varies among individuals and can be enhanced with practice. When exposed to a field of linearly polarized light, individuals can enhance their perception of Haidinger brushes by alternately covering each eye or gently rotating their heads, among other techniques.[1] These actions modulate the relative absorption of the orthogonal electric field components of the polarized light, aiding the perception of the characteristic "brush-like" pattern.
Movement of Haidinger brushes via head rotation can be beneficial for their perception. The intensity and clarity of the brushes also depend on the degree of polarization and the wavelength of the light, with the best perception reported under a high degree of polarization and at shorter wavelengths, as with blue light.[4] Light in the sky is mainly polarized due to Rayleigh scattering, is in its most polarized state at 90 degrees from the sun, and is most prominent at sunrise and sunset.[14] Reflection of the surface of the moon also results in polarized light.[14]
Etiological Theories of Haidinger Brushes
Although the exact physiological origin of Haidinger brushes remains to be fully elucidated, several prevailing theories attempt to explain their source. One point of agreement among these theories is the essential role of birefringent structures within the eye in facilitating the visibility of Haidinger brushes. Birefringence refers to the property of a material to refract light in two different directions. In the context of Haidinger brushes, this property is attributed to the macular pigment composed of xanthophylls, including lutein, zeaxanthin, and meso-zeaxanthin. Their specific macular location corresponds with the observed location and size of Haidinger brushes, along with the wavelength of light that results in the maximal perception of this visual phenomenon.[15]
In addition to the macular pigment, other structures in the eye have been proposed as possible contributors to the visibility of Haidinger brushes. Due to its polarization properties, the Henle fiber layer, a retinal layer around the fovea, may influence the perception of Haidinger brushes.[16] Some researchers also point towards the potential role of the birefringence of the cornea, along with the distribution and wave-guiding properties of short-wavelength cones or S-cones, in the manifestation of this entoptic phenomenon.[7] These theories, while divergent, shed light on the complexity of the physiological processes involved in the perception of Haidinger brushes.
Clinical Significance
Haidinger Brushes as a Marker for Macular Health
The perception of Haidinger brushes is intimately linked with the structural and functional integrity of the macula and, more specifically, the fovea. The macula, densely populated with cones, is responsible for high-resolution central vision. This detailed vision is critical for reading, driving, and recognizing faces.[17] The fovea is the central pit of the macula, where cones are most densely packed, and the xanthophyll pigment is found in high concentration.[18] Understanding the physiology of these areas is crucial for understanding Haidinger brushes as a marker for macular health. Changes in macular pigment density, such as those seen in age-related macular degeneration (AMD) or other retinal disorders and dystrophies, can influence the perception of Haidinger brushes.[4]
AMD, a common condition in older adults, leads to the loss of central vision. Dry AMD is characterized by the accumulation of drusen, extracellular deposits containing lipids and proteins that accumulate between the basal lamina of the retinal pigment epithelium (RPE) and the inner collagenous layer of the Bruch membrane.[19] The presence and growth of these drusen can lead to physical distortion and functional compromise of the macula, subsequently modifying the perception of Haidinger brushes.[20][4]
In addition to dry AMD, wet AMD and other retinal diseases can affect the perception of Haidinger Brushes. For instance, type 2 macular telangiectasia, a disease that induces abnormal blood vessel growth in the maculae, can alter macular function and, consequently, the perception of Haidinger brushes.[21] Stargardt disease, a form of inherited juvenile macular dystrophy, also impacts the macula by causing progressive vision loss due to the death of photoreceptor cells, which are vital for detecting light and color.[22] The macula can be affected by numerous other diseases, including other retinal dystrophies.[23] These alterations can change the perception of Haidinger brushes and signal the presence of these diseases.
Given the potential role of Haidinger brushes in the early detection of these diseases, they serve as a possible adjunct to the traditional suite of diagnostic tools available to ophthalmologists and optometrists. While the perception of Haidinger brushes may not provide a definitive diagnosis, it can offer a clue to underlying macular pathologies and potentially prompt a more comprehensive macular health evaluation.
Diagnostic Applications of Haidinger Brushes
Haidinger brushes have been shown to have intriguing diagnostic and prognostic applications beyond their known association with macular health. The clinical insights offered by the visualization of Haidinger brushes, or the lack thereof, has been explored in conditions like amblyopia and cataract.
In patients with amblyopia, a condition characterized by decreased vision in one or both eyes due to abnormal vision development in infancy or childhood, the perception of Haidinger brushes has been used in diagnostic and therapeutic protocols.[24] For instance, the visibility of Haidinger brushes can help assess the level of binocular function, which is often impaired in amblyopia. Researchers have also harnessed Haidinger brushes in pleoptic therapy, a treatment approach designed to improve visual acuity in the amblyopic eye by stimulating the underused eye and restoring some level of binocular vision.[25]
The perception of Haidinger brushes has been proposed as a prognostic marker for patients with cataracts. Cataracts, characterized by clouding of the usually clear crystalline lens of the eye, can cause blurry vision and difficulty with glare. Interestingly, studies suggest that the visibility of Haidinger brushes is minimally affected by the media opacity associated with cataracts. As such, the preoperative perception of Haidinger brushes might predict better postoperative visual outcomes.[7] In both scenarios, applying Haidinger brushes visualization acts as a noninvasive tool that could enhance the understanding, treatment, and monitoring of these common eye conditions in conjunction with standard diagnostic and prognostic methods.
Viewing Haidinger Brushes
Perceiving Haidinger brushes typically requires a specific viewing technique that capitalizes on the ability to discern polarized light. Most commonly, the technique involves the subject staring at a uniform bright white or blue field of polarized light, then shifting their gaze back and forth rotationally. This shifting gaze allows the observer to perceive the subtle contrast of the Haidinger brushes against the light field. This focused approach can enable their detection as Haidinger brushes are faint and typically not perceptible during regular vision.[4]
Light sources play a significant role in the ability to visualize Haidinger brushes. The light must be polarized, as the phenomenon hinges on the observed perception of this type of light. Natural daylight, which is partially polarized, can often allow the perception of Haidinger brushes, especially when reflected off certain surfaces like water or glass.[2] Artificial light sources, such as the light emitted from liquid crystal displays (LCD), also provide polarized light suitable for observing Haidinger brushes. LCD screens, used in most modern monitors and televisions, produce linearly polarized light when it passes through the liquid crystal layer.[26] This makes them a practical and accessible light source for observing Haidinger brushes. However, the observer must be careful to view the light at the correct angle, as the orientation of polarization from LCD screens can vary.
Inter-individual Variability in Perception of Haidinger Brushes
While the potential diagnostic utility of Haidinger brushes is notable, considering the inherent variability in their perception amongst different individuals is equally important. Even within a cohort of healthy subjects with no identifiable ocular pathology, the visibility of Haidinger brushes exhibits considerable variance.[27] This variability poses challenges in establishing definitive relationships between the perception of this entoptic phenomenon and specific physiological parameters. This variability in perception could be attributed to many factors, including differences in individual macular pigmentation, the density and arrangement of foveal cones, or even subjective differences in interpreting the visual phenomenon.[27] Despite this, the fact that Haidinger brushes can be observed in a subset of the healthy population and their absence or diminished perception can be associated with certain ocular pathologies underscores their potential diagnostic value.
Interactions With Color Vision Deficits
Haidinger brushes can be perceived by individuals with normal color vision and those with color vision deficits, such as those unable to pass the Ishihara color vision test. The perception of Haidinger brushes relies not on color perception but on the ability to discern the dichroism in the macular pigments, giving rise to the perception of polarized light. Even though the perception of Haidinger brushes is typically described as a yellowish bow-tie or propeller-like figure on a blue background, this is more a function of the absorbance spectrum of the macular pigments rather than the color perception abilities of the individual.[16] Despite this, color vision deficits might subtly influence the perception of Haidinger brushes; individuals with blue-yellow color blindness may perceive Haidinger brushes as less intense or differently colored. However, the full interplay between color vision deficits and the perception of Haidinger Brushes is not currently understood.
Nursing, Allied Health, and Interprofessional Team Interventions
In the realm of ocular healthcare, the principle of Haidinger brushes and understanding its basis in polarized light can significantly contribute to patient care when applied by a collaborative interprofessional healthcare team. Comprising ophthalmologists, optometrists, opticians, and optical technologists, this team harnesses the knowledge of polarized light in both lens selection and advanced imaging techniques.
Lens selection can benefit from understanding polarized light, which can reduce glare and increase visual comfort with polarizing filters, a particularly crucial factor for patients requiring precise visual acuity. Patients may receive more helpful eyewear by exploring polarization options with experienced team members.
Similarly, the utilization of polarized light in optical coherence tomography (OCT), a noninvasive imaging method, allows for the acquisition of detailed retinal images. While the connection of Haidinger brushes to the macular pigment raises the possibility of its use in the early detection of macular conditions such as age-related macular degeneration (AMD) and macular telangiectasia, this potential application requires further investigation. Nevertheless, the concept of Haidinger brushes may be introduced to patients as a noninvasive means of self-monitoring macular health, with the understanding that perception of Haidinger brushes can vary among healthy individuals.
Understanding Haidinger brushes and their potential implications in disease detection and patient education highlights the need for continued research. Through a coordinated effort, the interprofessional healthcare team can effectively implement current knowledge and adapt to future discoveries, ensuring patients receive the most appropriate and personalized ocular care.
Media
(Click Image to Enlarge)
(Click Image to Enlarge)
References
O'Shea RP, Misson GP, Temple SE. Seeing polarization of light with the naked eye. Current biology : CB. 2021 Feb 22:31(4):R178-R179. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2020.12.037. Epub [PubMed PMID: 33621501]
Misson GP, Timmerman BH, Bryanston-Cross PJ. Human perception of visual stimuli modulated by direction of linear polarization. Vision research. 2015 Oct:115(Pt A):48-57. doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2015.08.004. Epub 2015 Aug 28 [PubMed PMID: 26291073]
Temple SE, McGregor JE, Miles C, Graham L, Miller J, Buck J, Scott-Samuel NE, Roberts NW. Perceiving polarization with the naked eye: characterization of human polarization sensitivity. Proceedings. Biological sciences. 2015 Jul 22:282(1811):. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2015.0338. Epub [PubMed PMID: 26136441]
Temple SE, Roberts NW, Misson GP. Haidinger's brushes elicited at varying degrees of polarization rapidly and easily assesses total macular pigmentation. Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics, image science, and vision. 2019 Apr 1:36(4):B123-B131. doi: 10.1364/JOSAA.36.00B123. Epub [PubMed PMID: 31044990]
Kefalov VJ. Rod and cone visual pigments and phototransduction through pharmacological, genetic, and physiological approaches. The Journal of biological chemistry. 2012 Jan 13:287(3):1635-41. doi: 10.1074/jbc.R111.303008. Epub 2011 Nov 10 [PubMed PMID: 22074928]
Level 3 (low-level) evidenceYoshida T, Takagi Y, Igarashi-Yokoi T, Ohno-Matsui K. Efficacy of lutein supplements on macular pigment optical density in highly myopic individuals: A randomized controlled trial. Medicine. 2023 Mar 24:102(12):e33280. doi: 10.1097/MD.0000000000033280. Epub [PubMed PMID: 36961139]
Level 1 (high-level) evidenceMüller PL, Müller S, Gliem M, Küpper K, Holz FG, Harmening WM, Charbel Issa P. Perception of Haidinger Brushes in Macular Disease Depends on Macular Pigment Density and Visual Acuity. Investigative ophthalmology & visual science. 2016 Mar:57(3):1448-56. doi: 10.1167/iovs.15-19004. Epub [PubMed PMID: 27028066]
Li X, Holt RR, Keen CL, Morse LS, Zivkovic AM, Yiu G, Hackman RM. Potential roles of dietary zeaxanthin and lutein in macular health and function. Nutrition reviews. 2023 May 10:81(6):670-683. doi: 10.1093/nutrit/nuac076. Epub [PubMed PMID: 36094616]
Lem DW, Davey PG, Gierhart DL, Rosen RB. A Systematic Review of Carotenoids in the Management of Age-Related Macular Degeneration. Antioxidants (Basel, Switzerland). 2021 Aug 5:10(8):. doi: 10.3390/antiox10081255. Epub 2021 Aug 5 [PubMed PMID: 34439503]
Level 1 (high-level) evidenceHu W, Shankar P, Yao Y, Su X, Kim JE. Effect of xanthophyll-rich food and supplement intake on visual outcomes in healthy adults and those with eye disease: a systematic review, meta-analysis, and meta-regression of randomized controlled trials. Nutrition reviews. 2023 Apr 24:():. pii: nuad037. doi: 10.1093/nutrit/nuad037. Epub 2023 Apr 24 [PubMed PMID: 37094947]
Level 1 (high-level) evidenceBorkenstein AF, Borkenstein EM. Polarized glasses may help in symptomatic cases of intraocular lens glistenings. Clinical optometry. 2019:11():57-62. doi: 10.2147/OPTO.S202796. Epub 2019 May 6 [PubMed PMID: 31191065]
Level 3 (low-level) evidenceShaw JA, Vollmer M. Blue sun glints on water viewed through a polarizer. Applied optics. 2017 Jul 1:56(19):G36-G41. doi: 10.1364/AO.56.000G36. Epub [PubMed PMID: 29047467]
Dain SJ, Ngo TP, Cheng BB, Hu A, Teh AG, Tseng J, Vu N. Sunglasses, the European directive and the European standard. Ophthalmic & physiological optics : the journal of the British College of Ophthalmic Opticians (Optometrists). 2010 May:30(3):253-6. doi: 10.1111/j.1475-1313.2010.00711.x. Epub [PubMed PMID: 20444131]
Cronin TW, Marshall J. Patterns and properties of polarized light in air and water. Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences. 2011 Mar 12:366(1565):619-26. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2010.0201. Epub [PubMed PMID: 21282165]
Level 3 (low-level) evidenceMisson GP, Heitmar R, Armstrong R, Anderson SJ. The Differential Contribution of Macular Pigments and Foveal Anatomy to the Perception of Maxwell's Spot and Haidinger's Brushes. Vision (Basel, Switzerland). 2023 Feb 6:7(1):. doi: 10.3390/vision7010011. Epub 2023 Feb 6 [PubMed PMID: 36810315]
Misson GP, Temple SE, Anderson SJ. Polarization perception in humans: on the origin of and relationship between Maxwell's spot and Haidinger's brushes. Scientific reports. 2020 Jan 10:10(1):108. doi: 10.1038/s41598-019-56916-8. Epub 2020 Jan 10 [PubMed PMID: 31924831]
Bird AC, Bok D. Why the macula? Eye (London, England). 2018 May:32(5):858-862. doi: 10.1038/eye.2017.247. Epub 2017 Nov 17 [PubMed PMID: 29148528]
Tuten WS, Harmening WM. Foveal vision. Current biology : CB. 2021 Jun 7:31(11):R701-R703. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2021.03.097. Epub [PubMed PMID: 34102113]
Hernández-Zimbrón LF, Zamora-Alvarado R, Ochoa-De la Paz L, Velez-Montoya R, Zenteno E, Gulias-Cañizo R, Quiroz-Mercado H, Gonzalez-Salinas R. Age-Related Macular Degeneration: New Paradigms for Treatment and Management of AMD. Oxidative medicine and cellular longevity. 2018:2018():8374647. doi: 10.1155/2018/8374647. Epub 2018 Feb 1 [PubMed PMID: 29484106]
Zhang X, Sivaprasad S. Drusen and pachydrusen: the definition, pathogenesis, and clinical significance. Eye (London, England). 2021 Jan:35(1):121-133. doi: 10.1038/s41433-020-01265-4. Epub 2020 Nov 18 [PubMed PMID: 33208847]
Kedarisetti KC, Narayanan R, Stewart MW, Reddy Gurram N, Khanani AM. Macular Telangiectasia Type 2: A Comprehensive Review. Clinical ophthalmology (Auckland, N.Z.). 2022:16():3297-3309. doi: 10.2147/OPTH.S373538. Epub 2022 Oct 10 [PubMed PMID: 36237488]
Kohli P, Tripathy K, Kaur K. Stargardt Disease. StatPearls. 2024 Jan:(): [PubMed PMID: 36508525]
Georgiou M, Fujinami K, Michaelides M. Inherited retinal diseases: Therapeutics, clinical trials and end points-A review. Clinical & experimental ophthalmology. 2021 Apr:49(3):270-288. doi: 10.1111/ceo.13917. Epub 2021 Mar 20 [PubMed PMID: 33686777]
Verma A, Singh D. Active vision therapy for pseudophakic amblyopia. Journal of cataract and refractive surgery. 1997 Sep:23(7):1089-94 [PubMed PMID: 9379383]
Kelly N, Vukicevic M, Koklanis K. Effectiveness of visual and acoustic biofeedback eccentric viewing training in conjunction with home exercises on visual function: a retrospective observational review. Strabismus. 2023 Mar:31(1):55-65. doi: 10.1080/09273972.2023.2172435. Epub 2023 Mar 13 [PubMed PMID: 36908278]
Level 2 (mid-level) evidenceNiu R, Zhang C, Li X, Ma H, Sun Y. Achieving a wide color gamut based on polarization interference filters in a liquid crystal display. Optics express. 2022 Sep 26:30(20):36155-36166. doi: 10.1364/OE.467870. Epub [PubMed PMID: 36258551]
Mottes J, Ortolan D, Ruffato G. Haidinger's brushes: Psychophysical analysis of an entoptic phenomenon. Vision research. 2022 Oct:199():108076. doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2022.108076. Epub 2022 Jun 13 [PubMed PMID: 35709591]