Back To Search Results

Intraoperative and Anesthesia Awareness

Editor: David N. Flynn Updated: 1/29/2023 9:25:21 AM

Introduction

"The knife is searching for disease, the pulleys are dragging back dislocated limbs, nature herself is working out the primal curse which doomed the tenderest of her creatures to the sharpest of her trials, but the fierce extremity of suffering has been steeped in the waters of forgetfulness, and the deepest furrow in the knotted brow of agony has been smoothed forever."  

Dr. Oliver Wendell Holmes, Sr., uttered those words almost 200 years ago, in November of 1846, commenting in a lecture to students at Massachusetts Medical College on the first public demonstration of ether just a month before. The historical event has since become known as "Ether Day." Indeed, when Dr William Morton, a dentist, successfully anesthetized Mr. Gilbert Abbot with ether to resect a tuberculous submandibular lesion in the amphitheater of Massachusetts General Hospital, many shared this belief. Corresponding with Dr. Morton shortly after that, Dr. Holmes is believed to have coined the term "anesthesia," stating:

"Everybody wants to have a hand in a great discovery. All I will do is give a hint or two as to names—or the name—to be applied to the state produced and the agent. The state should, I think, be called "Anaesthesia." This signifies insensibility—more particularly...to objects of touch." 

The medical community assumed then that humanity's unavoidable suffering through surgical intervention—the "primal curse" of humankind—was relegated to the dustbin of medical history. And yet, in the third decade of the 21st century, intraoperative awareness continues to threaten patients with horror and agony on the operating table, even as expectations for painless surgery have become commonplace and as practitioners of anesthesia continue to minimize both its frequency and relevance. 

This review addresses incidence, risk factors, anesthetic depth monitoring, prevention strategies, and perioperative, team-based management of unintended intraoperative awareness. The topic concludes with a brief review of the long-term effects of accidental awareness during general anesthesia (AAGA) on patients and practitioners. 

Intraoperative awareness is characterized by the coincidence of intraoperative consciousness and explicit, episodic postoperative recall of events during a planned anesthetic.[1] Patient experiences may range from isolated auditory recall to the catastrophic experience of painful surgical stimulation combined with the sensation of suffocation and paralysis in the setting of neuromuscular blockade (NMB).[2] While the broader phenomenon of intraoperative awareness may occur in any setting where a patient expects a depressed level of consciousness, including in regional anesthesia, procedural, or intravenous (IV) sedation, most attention—both from patients and practitioners- is focused on AAGA. Estimates of the incidence of AAGA range from 1:1000 to 1:20,000, with a wide range of variation dependent upon variables in anesthetic technique, patient population, and surgical context/procedure.[3][4] Notably, published incidence rates are highly dependent on the investigational approach, with rates derived from directed postoperative questioning far exceeding those in studies of self-reported awareness.

Issues of Concern

Register For Free And Read The Full Article
Get the answers you need instantly with the StatPearls Clinical Decision Support tool. StatPearls spent the last decade developing the largest and most updated Point-of Care resource ever developed. Earn CME/CE by searching and reading articles.
  • Dropdown arrow Search engine and full access to all medical articles
  • Dropdown arrow 10 free questions in your specialty
  • Dropdown arrow Free CME/CE Activities
  • Dropdown arrow Free daily question in your email
  • Dropdown arrow Save favorite articles to your dashboard
  • Dropdown arrow Emails offering discounts

Learn more about a Subscription to StatPearls Point-of-Care

Issues of Concern

Risk Factors

Risk factors for AAGA may be broadly separated into those related to 1) technical application of anesthetics, 2) surgical procedure or context, and 3) inherent patient characteristics. 

Risk Factors Related to Anesthetic Technique 

Among the risk factors for anesthetic techniques, NMB is simultaneously the most significant and modifiable. The vast majority of self-reported cases of AAGA in the 5th National Audit Project for Awareness (NAP5) were associated with the use of NMBs (incidence 1:8200 with NMB vs. 1:135900 without NMB); this represents a more than 16-fold increase of reported instances where NMBs were used.[4] By disrupting the neuromuscular junction, NMBs deprive the practitioner of the most reliable indicator of insufficient or “light” anesthesia: patient movement. Furthermore, the experience of paralysis is novel to the patient and deeply distressing, adding profound psychological impact to episodes of auditory, tactile, or painful perception and recall. The significance of psychological distress in the context of awareness is recognized in the Michigan Awareness Classification Instrument, where such instances are notated with a “D” modifier.[2] 

Total intravenous anesthesia (TIVA) is also associated with an increased risk of AAGA compared to volatile-based anesthetics.[4][5] Contributing factors are rooted in the inherent mode of delivery of IV anesthetics, resulting in the absence of a reliable means to monitor anesthetic concentrations and the myriad opportunities for disruption of delivery of an intravenous infusion. Such examples include human error in pump programming, disconnected or unconnected infusion tubing, and unrecognized peripheral IV (PIV) site infiltration. Mechanical failure of drug delivery is often compounded by PIV sites that are tucked, draped, or otherwise obscured from the continuous view of the practitioner. Risk mitigation with TIVA may theoretically be achieved by target-controlled infusions (TCI). However, no evidence suggests that AAGA occurs at lower rates when TCI is utilized to deliver TIVA. Moreover, TCIs are not presently approved by the FDA for use in the United States. Processed electroencephalogram (EEG) monitoring (bispectral index or BIS) may significantly detect unintended consciousness during TIVA general anesthetics. As such, its use is recommended for cases where volatile concentrations are deemed insufficient to provide reliable unconsciousness and amnestic effects.[5][6]

Problems with the technical delivery of anesthetics contribute to AAGA, though human error is more often the culprit than the failure of a mechanical or electronic device.[7] Examples include failure to turn on or refill an empty vaporizer or errors in pump programming. 

Risk Factors Related to Surgical Procedure or Context 

Risk factors for AAGA related to surgical procedure or context are easily understood when one considers the common characteristics: urgent or emergent procedures, rapid pace from induction to surgical incision, and the attendant objectives that necessitate reduction of anesthetics in 1 form or another (eg, trauma, hypovolemia, depressed cardiac function.) Each of these characteristics is independent of and, in addition to the added risk when NMBs are employed, as described above. 

Emergency surgery, particularly trauma and cesarean section, poses relatively higher risks of AAGA due primarily to the reduction of anesthetic doses that are often required in these situations.[8][9] In the setting of emergent trauma, blood loss, and subsequent hemodynamic instability often preclude the delivery of anesthetics that are sufficient to prevent awareness and recall, as potent anesthetics are commonly associated with decreases in both myocardial contractility and systemic vascular resistance. Emergent cesarean delivery necessitates a rapid pace from identification of fetal or maternal distress to surgical delivery, resulting in scenarios where adequate anesthetic concentrations may not be achieved at the time of surgical incision. Additionally, anesthetics are often intentionally held during cesarean section to lower concentrations even when the interval between induction and delivery is longer, as anesthetics easily cross the placenta and may result in both respiratory and circulatory depression in the fetus.[9][10][11] 

Common to both the emergent trauma and obstetric populations, the potential for difficult airway placement introduces further opportunities for unintended awareness. Intravenous induction agents rapidly redistribute, resulting in lower blood and brain concentrations. At the same time, alveolar delivery of inhaled anesthetics is delayed by definitive airway placement, and necessary replacement with IV agents may be delayed amidst the focus on obtaining airway control.[11]

Cardiopulmonary bypass (CPB) also increases the risk of AAGA. This increase in risk may be related to the altered pharmacodynamics of bypass physiology.[12] Historically, open-heart surgery has been recognized to foster recall—though often painless—of intraoperative events due to the widespread use of opioid-based anesthetic techniques in favor of more hemodynamically compromising volatile-based techniques that have since become more common in recent decades.[13][14]

Patient-Related Risk Factors 

Patients reporting a history of intraoperative awareness may be at higher risk for subsequent AAGA, as reported by at least 1 observational study.[15]  Whether or not further studies validate an increased risk in this setting, patients who report a prior history of awareness deserve an appropriate and focused preoperative evaluation on discerning the context and nature of the episode where possible. Importantly, any planned risk-mitigation strategies that may lower the likelihood of similar patient experiences should be shared with the patient in advance to validate concerns and assuage any attendant anxiety. 

While not definitively proven or identified, genetic resistance to anesthetics may contribute to increased requirements and risk of AAGA.[16]

Chronic substance abuse may contribute to acquired resistance to anesthetics. Long-term use of commonly abused substances, including alcohol, opioids, and benzodiazepines, may increase anesthetic requirements, increasing the risk of AAGA.[17] Acute intoxication with the same substances may result in CNS depression, lowering anesthetic requirements. These counterbalancing effects must be considered when assessing the risk for AAGA. An extensive list of medications not typically associated with abuse and dependence may also raise anesthetic requirements by inducing cytochrome P450 3A.[18] Commonly encountered examples include barbiturates, phenytoin, carbamazepine, and glucocorticoids. 

AAGA is shown to be most common at induction and emergence from general anesthesia.[19] Difficult airway placement prolongs the induction phase, complicates and often delays the delivery of inhaled volatile anesthetics, and increases the likelihood of unintended awareness. Studies indicating a higher risk of AAGA in the obese population may stem from the related incidence of difficult airway management within this cohort.[20][21]

In addition to acute hemodynamic compromise, patients presenting with chronic cardiovascular conditions such as congestive heart failure or significant valvular disease are also shown to be at higher risk for AAGA, as dose reduction strategies are commonly employed to maintain hemodynamic stability in these settings.

Monitoring Depth of Anesthesia and Prevention of AAGA 

Monitoring for prevention of AAGA rests upon 3 primary axes of data: 1) end-tidal anesthetic concentration (ETAC), 2) blockade of the neuromuscular junction with NMB, and 3) brain monitoring with raw or processed EEG. Physiologic responses to surgical stimulation, including heart rate, blood pressure, and purposeful movement, are monitored as routine components of anesthetic care. However, the reliability of physiologic parameters as an indicator of AAGA is often compromised by variability in sympathetic responses, beta- or calcium-channel blockade, or, in the case of patient movement, NMB. 

Monitoring ETAC is a basic standard of modern anesthetic delivery.[22] Any given volatile agent's minimum alveolar concentration (MAC) prevents movement with a surgical stimulus in 50% of the population.[23] The MAC-awake or the MAC required to suppress consciousness is often lower than the concentration required to prevent movement, which inherently protects against AAGA when MAC levels are achieved.[24] MAC varies significantly across ages, comorbidities, and other patient and environmental factors.[25][26][27][28] Given this variability, the coadministration of NMB with volatile anesthetics may complicate the interpretation of safe MAC levels to prevent AAGA, as patient movement is abolished. MAC is only available in inhaled volatile anesthetics and, therefore, irrelevant when TIVA is utilized. 

Monitoring the depth of NMB is critical to avoid unnecessary intraoperative paralysis and adequate recovery of the neuromuscular junction (NMJ) at emergence from anesthesia. Most surgery may be performed with reasonable and intermittent use of NMB rather than a persistent state of complete paralysis.[29] Indeed, many surgeries do not require NMB at all. To minimize unnecessary use of NMBs, consideration for the requirement (or not) of NMB should occur before the patient's induction and include all perioperative team members. If paralysis is a surgical requirement, appropriate monitoring of the NMJ is essential to ensure that the minimal amount of NMB required is utilized to achieve surgical objectives. AAGA is known to occur as a byproduct of post-emergence neuromuscular weakness, further strengthening the case for adequate monitoring and reversal where NMBs are used.[29]

Processed EEG monitors are commonly employed to assess the level of consciousness under GA. One such common monitor, the BIS monitor, translates raw EEG data into frequency bands that reflect a numeric value from 0 to 100. One randomized study demonstrated that patients targeted between a BIS value of 40 and 60 were less likely to experience AAGA. Notably, no studies have shown an advantage of processed EEG monitoring over standard monitoring of ETAC concentration when available, including large meta-analyses.[30] Processed EEG suffers from several limitations, most significantly that values are not tightly correlated to ETAC concentration and that non-anesthetic agents may alter processed EEG values independent of the level of consciousness. NMB alone, by decreasing or eliminating the electrical signals generated by contracting muscles, has decreased BIS values when administered without any anesthetics.[31] Based on current evidence, processed EEG monitoring should not substitute for a holistic, comprehensive, and vigilant approach to patient monitoring for depth of consciousness using all available physiologic and anesthetic parameters. 

Raw EEG possesses a significant advantage over processed EEG when monitoring for depth of consciousness. EEG waveforms are altered consistently and reproducibly when anesthetics are present at clinically relevant concentrations.[32] However, the utility of raw EEG monitoring is significantly limited due to the relative inability of most practitioners to accurately interpret raw EEG data, thus creating a significant market for processed EEG monitors, however useful they may or may not be in detecting levels of consciousness.[30][33]

Clinical Significance

Management of AAGA 

When AAGA, or any form of awareness associated with patient distress, is suspected or confirmed, a prompt and multi-disciplinary proactive approach to management is the standard of care.[22] The ASA guidelines suggest immediate administration of a benzodiazepine if awareness is suspected during the procedure. While potentially helpful, it must be recognized that midazolam, lorazepam, and similar agents are unreliable in preventing retrograde recall.[34] Even when administered preoperatively or in advance of the event, the degree of pain, paralysis, or distress from other stimuli may be sufficient to outweigh the sole administration of a benzodiazepine. More important is the immediate deepening of anesthetic agents when clinical signs and symptoms suggest intraoperative AAGA. 

Postoperatively, patients with a suspected awareness event should be interviewed at several intervals to assess and minimize the downstream psychological impact of AAGA. Immediate assessment in the PACU, either in response to a patient report of awareness or when suspected by a clinician, should focus on an open-ended discussion whereby the patient can relate their experience in detail. Dismissal of related events or justification for awareness from the practitioner at this point is inadvisable, and it is paramount to validate the concerns raised by the patient early in their recovery. Expressing regret for unpleasant experiences is ethically sound and vital for patients to hear. Expression of regret for untoward outcomes, and in many states, even apologizing, cannot be used against a practitioner as evidence of negligence in a malpractice claim. 

Immediate post-operative interviews should occur in the presence of surgical and anesthesiology team members to ensure that all parties are informed of events and that a shared understanding and analysis of causative factors may be undertaken. A patient’s recognition of awareness may be delayed by hours or even days. In many cases, patients may hesitate to initiate conversations about unintended awareness until asked.[35] In such cases, specific and validated questions are essential tools to assess the likelihood of AAGA.

The Brice questionnaire, first described by Brice, Hetherington, and Utting in 1970, attempts to solicit experiences consistent with awareness by distinguishing explicit recall from dreaming and is open-ended in both format and syntax.[36] When employed on a routine basis postoperatively between days 28 and 30, this questionnaire is superior to standard quality assurance follow-ups currently used in surgical facilities. In 1 study capturing 18,836 patients, 19 cases of AAGA were revealed with the Brice questionnaire 1 month out from surgery, yielding a rate of awareness of 0.1%, consistent with previous studies.[37] 

In contrast, only 3 of those 19 individuals were captured on the basic post-operative quality assurance calls, demonstrating that commonly employed follow-up methods are insufficient tools to screen for AAGA. Directed interviews 1 month out for all surgical patients pose a logistical challenge for healthcare infrastructure, particularly in a depleted and overburdened workforce. Herein lies a considerable and persistent challenge in accurately detecting AAGA events. 

Effect of Accidental Awareness on Patients 

Awareness events are commonly associated with psychological sequela. Nightmares, depression, anxiety, and PTSD are reported at significant but variable rates after AAGA.[38][39][40][39] Avoidance of medical care is a common downstream consequence of significant awareness events and may lead to significant long-term health consequences. Of note, cases of AAGA involving paralysis have been shown to lead to more frequent long-term psychological distress.[41] Importantly, psychological sequelae are also common among self-reported cases of awareness after IV sedation or regional anesthetic techniques; despite that, a practitioner may anticipate and even plan for fluctuating levels of consciousness wherever general anesthesia is not employed.[39]

As such, it is of critical importance to perform a detailed and comprehensive informed consent for the planned anesthetic, including the possibility of awareness and explicit recall, even in the setting of routine preoperative benzodiazepine administration. Indeed, significant gaps in understanding among patients of commonly performed sedation and anesthetic techniques may be identified during a properly informed consent process, thus offering an opportunity to educate the patient and manage expectations for planned consciousness or the lack thereof. Psychiatric or psychologic referral and treatment are highly recommended after suspected or confirmed awareness events.[22][41] Appropriate evaluation and treatment by mental health professionals play a significant role in reducing long-term detrimental effects on the psychiatric and physical well-being of affected individuals.

Enhancing Healthcare Team Outcomes

An interprofessional team of anesthesiologists, nurse anesthetists, surgeons, and perioperative nurses must understand the patient and the anesthetic and procedural risk factors that predispose patients to accidental awareness. Risks can be mitigated through avoidance or judicious use of muscle relaxants, depth of consciousness monitoring, and administration of benzodiazepines when clinically appropriate. When patients experience accidental awareness, structured interviews should be performed, and referral for psychological support should be offered.

References


[1]

Mashour GA, Orser BA, Avidan MS. Intraoperative awareness: from neurobiology to clinical practice. Anesthesiology. 2011 May:114(5):1218-33. doi: 10.1097/ALN.0b013e31820fc9b6. Epub     [PubMed PMID: 21464699]


[2]

Mashour GA, Esaki RK, Tremper KK, Glick DB, O'Connor M, Avidan MS. A novel classification instrument for intraoperative awareness events. Anesthesia and analgesia. 2010 Mar 1:110(3):813-5. doi: 10.1213/ANE.0b013e3181b6267d. Epub 2009 Aug 27     [PubMed PMID: 19713251]


[3]

Sebel PS, Bowdle TA, Ghoneim MM, Rampil IJ, Padilla RE, Gan TJ, Domino KB. The incidence of awareness during anesthesia: a multicenter United States study. Anesthesia and analgesia. 2004 Sep:99(3):833-839. doi: 10.1213/01.ANE.0000130261.90896.6C. Epub     [PubMed PMID: 15333419]

Level 2 (mid-level) evidence

[4]

Pandit JJ, Andrade J, Bogod DG, Hitchman JM, Jonker WR, Lucas N, Mackay JH, Nimmo AF, O'Connor K, O'Sullivan EP, Paul RG, Palmer JH, Plaat F, Radcliffe JJ, Sury MR, Torevell HE, Wang M, Hainsworth J, Cook TM, Royal College of Anaesthetists, Association of Anaesthetists of Great Britain and Ireland. 5th National Audit Project (NAP5) on accidental awareness during general anaesthesia: summary of main findings and risk factors. British journal of anaesthesia. 2014 Oct:113(4):549-59. doi: 10.1093/bja/aeu313. Epub 2014 Sep 9     [PubMed PMID: 25204697]


[5]

Zhang C, Xu L, Ma YQ, Sun YX, Li YH, Zhang L, Feng CS, Luo B, Zhao ZL, Guo JR, Jin YJ, Wu G, Yuan W, Yuan ZG, Yue Y. Bispectral index monitoring prevent awareness during total intravenous anesthesia: a prospective, randomized, double-blinded, multi-center controlled trial. Chinese medical journal. 2011 Nov:124(22):3664-9     [PubMed PMID: 22340221]

Level 1 (high-level) evidence

[6]

Avidan MS, Jacobsohn E, Glick D, Burnside BA, Zhang L, Villafranca A, Karl L, Kamal S, Torres B, O'Connor M, Evers AS, Gradwohl S, Lin N, Palanca BJ, Mashour GA, BAG-RECALL Research Group. Prevention of intraoperative awareness in a high-risk surgical population. The New England journal of medicine. 2011 Aug 18:365(7):591-600. doi: 10.1056/NEJMoa1100403. Epub     [PubMed PMID: 21848460]

Level 1 (high-level) evidence

[7]

Sivia DS, Pandit JJ. Mathematical model of the risk of drug error during anaesthesia: the influence of drug choices, injection routes, operation duration and fatigue. Anaesthesia. 2019 Aug:74(8):992-1000. doi: 10.1111/anae.14629. Epub 2019 Mar 18     [PubMed PMID: 30883682]


[8]

Errando CL, Sigl JC, Robles M, Calabuig E, García J, Arocas F, Higueras R, Del Rosario E, López D, Peiró CM, Soriano JL, Chaves S, Gil F, García-Aguado R. Awareness with recall during general anaesthesia: a prospective observational evaluation of 4001 patients. British journal of anaesthesia. 2008 Aug:101(2):178-85. doi: 10.1093/bja/aen144. Epub 2008 May 30     [PubMed PMID: 18515816]


[9]

Ghoneim MM, Block RI, Haffarnan M, Mathews MJ. Awareness during anesthesia: risk factors, causes and sequelae: a review of reported cases in the literature. Anesthesia and analgesia. 2009 Feb:108(2):527-35. doi: 10.1213/ane.0b013e318193c634. Epub     [PubMed PMID: 19151283]

Level 1 (high-level) evidence

[10]

Odor PM, Bampoe S, Lucas DN, Moonesinghe SR, Andrade J, Pandit JJ, DREAMY Investigators Group. Protocol for direct reporting of awareness in maternity patients (DREAMY): a prospective, multicentre cohort study of accidental awareness during general anaesthesia. International journal of obstetric anesthesia. 2020 May:42():47-56. doi: 10.1016/j.ijoa.2020.02.004. Epub 2020 Feb 14     [PubMed PMID: 32139144]


[11]

Odor PM, Bampoe S, Moonesinghe SR, Andrade J, Pandit JJ, Lucas DN, Pan-London Perioperative Audit and Research Network (PLAN), for the DREAMY Investigators Group. General anaesthetic and airway management practice for obstetric surgery in England: a prospective, multicentre observational study. Anaesthesia. 2021 Apr:76(4):460-471. doi: 10.1111/anae.15250. Epub 2020 Sep 21     [PubMed PMID: 32959372]

Level 2 (mid-level) evidence

[12]

Yeoh CJ, Hwang NC. Volatile Anesthesia Versus Total Intravenous Anesthesia During Cardiopulmonary Bypass: A Narrative Review on the Technical Challenges and Considerations. Journal of cardiothoracic and vascular anesthesia. 2020 Aug:34(8):2181-2188. doi: 10.1053/j.jvca.2020.03.013. Epub 2020 Mar 19     [PubMed PMID: 32360007]

Level 3 (low-level) evidence

[13]

Wang Y, Yue Y, Sun YH, Wu AS, Wu QW, Zhang YQ, Feng CS. Investigation and analysis of incidence of awareness in patients undergoing cardiac surgery in Beijing, China. Chinese medical journal. 2005 Jul 20:118(14):1190-4     [PubMed PMID: 16117864]


[14]

Freiermuth D, Mets B, Bolliger D, Reuthebuch O, Doebele T, Scholz M, Gregor M, Haschke M, Seeberger M, Fassl J. Sevoflurane and Isoflurane-Pharmacokinetics, Hemodynamic Stability and Cardio-protective Effects During Cardiopulmonary Bypass. Journal of cardiothoracic and vascular anesthesia. 2017 Dec:31(6):e85-e86. doi: 10.1053/j.jvca.2016.10.010. Epub 2016 Oct 17     [PubMed PMID: 28057406]


[15]

Aranake A, Gradwohl S, Ben-Abdallah A, Lin N, Shanks A, Helsten DL, Glick DB, Jacobsohn E, Villafranca AJ, Evers AS, Avidan MS, Mashour GA. Increased risk of intraoperative awareness in patients with a history of awareness. Anesthesiology. 2013 Dec:119(6):1275-83. doi: 10.1097/ALN.0000000000000023. Epub     [PubMed PMID: 24113645]

Level 2 (mid-level) evidence

[16]

Sleigh JW, Leslie K, Davidson AJ, Amor DJ, Diakumis P, Lukic V, Lockhart PJ, Bahlo M. Genetic Analysis of Patients Who Experienced Awareness with Recall while under General Anesthesia. Anesthesiology. 2019 Nov:131(5):974-982. doi: 10.1097/ALN.0000000000002877. Epub     [PubMed PMID: 31335548]


[17]

Vadivelu N, Mitra S, Kaye AD, Urman RD. Perioperative analgesia and challenges in the drug-addicted and drug-dependent patient. Best practice & research. Clinical anaesthesiology. 2014 Mar:28(1):91-101. doi: 10.1016/j.bpa.2014.02.003. Epub 2014 Mar 18     [PubMed PMID: 24815969]


[18]

Flockhart DA, Oesterheld JR. Cytochrome P450-mediated drug interactions. Child and adolescent psychiatric clinics of North America. 2000 Jan:9(1):43-76     [PubMed PMID: 10674190]

Level 3 (low-level) evidence

[19]

Odor PM, Bampoe S, Lucas DN, Moonesinghe SR, Andrade J, Pandit JJ, Pan-London Peri-operative Audit and Research Network (PLAN), for the DREAMY Investigators Group. Incidence of accidental awareness during general anaesthesia in obstetrics: a multicentre, prospective cohort study. Anaesthesia. 2021 Jun:76(6):759-776. doi: 10.1111/anae.15385. Epub 2021 Jan 12     [PubMed PMID: 33434945]


[20]

Palmer J,Sury M,Nightingale C,Pandit J,Cook T,NAP5 group collaboration., Underestimating the size of the problem? UK anaesthesia for patients with obesity. Obesity research     [PubMed PMID: 28629668]


[21]

Members of the Working Party, Nightingale CE, Margarson MP, Shearer E, Redman JW, Lucas DN, Cousins JM, Fox WT, Kennedy NJ, Venn PJ, Skues M, Gabbott D, Misra U, Pandit JJ, Popat MT, Griffiths R, Association of Anaesthetists of Great Britain, Ireland Society for Obesity and Bariatric Anaesthesia. Peri-operative management of the obese surgical patient 2015: Association of Anaesthetists of Great Britain and Ireland Society for Obesity and Bariatric Anaesthesia. Anaesthesia. 2015 Jul:70(7):859-76. doi: 10.1111/anae.13101. Epub 2015 May 7     [PubMed PMID: 25950621]


[22]

American Society of Anesthesiologists Task Force on Intraoperative Awareness. Practice advisory for intraoperative awareness and brain function monitoring: a report by the american society of anesthesiologists task force on intraoperative awareness. Anesthesiology. 2006 Apr:104(4):847-64     [PubMed PMID: 16571982]


[23]

Eger EI 2nd, Saidman LJ, Brandstater B. Minimum alveolar anesthetic concentration: a standard of anesthetic potency. Anesthesiology. 1965 Nov-Dec:26(6):756-63     [PubMed PMID: 5844267]

Level 3 (low-level) evidence

[24]

Antognini JF, Schwartz K. Exaggerated anesthetic requirements in the preferentially anesthetized brain. Anesthesiology. 1993 Dec:79(6):1244-9     [PubMed PMID: 8267200]

Level 3 (low-level) evidence

[25]

Liem EB, Lin CM, Suleman MI, Doufas AG, Gregg RG, Veauthier JM, Loyd G, Sessler DI. Anesthetic requirement is increased in redheads. Anesthesiology. 2004 Aug:101(2):279-83     [PubMed PMID: 15277908]


[26]

Eger EI 2nd. Age, minimum alveolar anesthetic concentration, and minimum alveolar anesthetic concentration-awake. Anesthesia and analgesia. 2001 Oct:93(4):947-53     [PubMed PMID: 11574362]


[27]

Nickalls RW,Mapleson WW, Age-related iso-MAC charts for isoflurane, sevoflurane and desflurane in man. British journal of anaesthesia. 2003 Aug;     [PubMed PMID: 12878613]


[28]

Kanazawa S, Oda Y, Maeda C, Okutani R. Age-dependent decrease in desflurane concentration for maintaining bispectral index below 50. Acta anaesthesiologica Scandinavica. 2016 Feb:60(2):177-82. doi: 10.1111/aas.12642. Epub 2015 Oct 12     [PubMed PMID: 26459260]


[29]

Tasbihgou SR, Vogels MF, Absalom AR. Accidental awareness during general anaesthesia - a narrative review. Anaesthesia. 2018 Jan:73(1):112-122. doi: 10.1111/anae.14124. Epub     [PubMed PMID: 29210043]

Level 3 (low-level) evidence

[30]

Punjasawadwong Y, Phongchiewboon A, Bunchungmongkol N. Bispectral index for improving anaesthetic delivery and postoperative recovery. The Cochrane database of systematic reviews. 2014 Jun 17:2014(6):CD003843. doi: 10.1002/14651858.CD003843.pub3. Epub 2014 Jun 17     [PubMed PMID: 24937564]

Level 1 (high-level) evidence

[31]

Schuller PJ, Newell S, Strickland PA, Barry JJ. Response of bispectral index to neuromuscular block in awake volunteers. British journal of anaesthesia. 2015 Jul:115 Suppl 1():i95-i103. doi: 10.1093/bja/aev072. Epub     [PubMed PMID: 26174308]


[32]

Reshef ER, Schiff ND, Brown EN. A Neurologic Examination for Anesthesiologists: Assessing Arousal Level during Induction, Maintenance, and Emergence. Anesthesiology. 2019 Mar:130(3):462-471. doi: 10.1097/ALN.0000000000002559. Epub     [PubMed PMID: 30664547]


[33]

Bennett C, Voss LJ, Barnard JP, Sleigh JW. Practical use of the raw electroencephalogram waveform during general anesthesia: the art and science. Anesthesia and analgesia. 2009 Aug:109(2):539-50. doi: 10.1213/ane.0b013e3181a9fc38. Epub     [PubMed PMID: 19608830]


[34]

Bulach R, Myles PS, Russnak M. Double-blind randomized controlled trial to determine extent of amnesia with midazolam given immediately before general anaesthesia. British journal of anaesthesia. 2005 Mar:94(3):300-5     [PubMed PMID: 15567810]

Level 1 (high-level) evidence

[35]

Moerman N, Bonke B, Oosting J. Awareness and recall during general anesthesia. Facts and feelings. Anesthesiology. 1993 Sep:79(3):454-64     [PubMed PMID: 8363069]


[36]

Brice DD,Hetherington RR,Utting JE, A simple study of awareness and dreaming during anaesthesia. British journal of anaesthesia. 1970 Jun;     [PubMed PMID: 5423844]


[37]

Mashour GA, Kent C, Picton P, Ramachandran SK, Tremper KK, Turner CR, Shanks A, Avidan MS. Assessment of intraoperative awareness with explicit recall: a comparison of 2 methods. Anesthesia and analgesia. 2013 Apr:116(4):889-91. doi: 10.1213/ANE.0b013e318281e9ad. Epub 2013 Mar 4     [PubMed PMID: 23460567]

Level 2 (mid-level) evidence

[38]

Whitlock EL, Rodebaugh TL, Hassett AL, Shanks AM, Kolarik E, Houghtby J, West HM, Burnside BA, Shumaker E, Villafranca A, Edwards WA, Levinson CA, Langer JK, Fernandez KC, El-Gabalawy R, Zhou EY, Sareen J, Jacobsohn E, Mashour GA, Avidan MS. Psychological sequelae of surgery in a prospective cohort of patients from three intraoperative awareness prevention trials. Anesthesia and analgesia. 2015 Jan:120(1):87-95. doi: 10.1213/ANE.0000000000000498. Epub     [PubMed PMID: 25383719]

Level 2 (mid-level) evidence

[39]

Kent CD, Mashour GA, Metzger NA, Posner KL, Domino KB. Psychological impact of unexpected explicit recall of events occurring during surgery performed under sedation, regional anaesthesia, and general anaesthesia: data from the Anesthesia Awareness Registry. British journal of anaesthesia. 2013 Mar:110(3):381-7. doi: 10.1093/bja/aes386. Epub 2012 Nov 15     [PubMed PMID: 23161356]


[40]

Bruchas RR, Kent CD, Wilson HD, Domino KB. Anesthesia awareness: narrative review of psychological sequelae, treatment, and incidence. Journal of clinical psychology in medical settings. 2011 Sep:18(3):257-67. doi: 10.1007/s10880-011-9233-8. Epub     [PubMed PMID: 21512752]

Level 3 (low-level) evidence

[41]

Cook TM, Andrade J, Bogod DG, Hitchman JM, Jonker WR, Lucas N, Mackay JH, Nimmo AF, O'Connor K, O'Sullivan EP, Paul RG, Palmer JH, Plaat F, Radcliffe JJ, Sury MR, Torevell HE, Wang M, Hainsworth J, Pandit JJ, Royal College of Anaesthetists and the Association of Anaesthetists of Great Britain and Ireland. The 5th National Audit Project (NAP5) on accidental awareness during general anaesthesia: patient experiences, human factors, sedation, consent and medicolegal issues. Anaesthesia. 2014 Oct:69(10):1102-16. doi: 10.1111/anae.12827. Epub     [PubMed PMID: 25204237]